write applies Understanding sentence structures

Understanding sentence structures

In this article about English syntax we look at the common order of constituents in sentences. Following this, we analyse the reasons why writers may choose to structure their sentences differently and the many techniques that they have for doing so.

Introduction

Readers expect information to be presented to them sentence-by-sentence in certain constituent orders. For example, they expect the (S)ubject constituent to come first and be followed by the (V)erb constituent, which in turn is followed by the (O)bject constituent (if there is one).

This fundamental expectation is the basis for the classification of English as an SVO language.

Readers also expect information to be presented to them according to certain general organisational principles. One of these principles is that the new information in a sentence will come at the end of the sentence.

In most cases in English, the specific constituent orders align with the general organisational principles. So there is usually no problem. But when they do come into conflict, writers will most often choose to prioritise one or more of the general organisational principles over the expected constituent order. And they have several specific techniques for doing so.

On this basis, the rest of the article is organised as follows:

Note: Throughout this article I refer to writers and readers. But the points made apply equally to speakers and listeners.

Sentence constituents

We have noted that English is classified as an SVO language, with the three constituents being the subject, verb and object. The other two sentence† constituents are the (C)omplement and the (A)djunct (also known as an (A)dverbial).

There are no simple but fully adequate definitions of the constituents. So, let's just look at some examples as being sufficiently explanatory.

Subject:
He has been learning English for three years.
The government plans to increase taxes.
The girl who won the chess competition comes from Korea.
Verb:
I think you are wrong.
Thousands attended the street protests at the weekend.
The school is planning to extend online teaching next year.
Object (direct):
Did you eat the last cookie?
My sister always beats me at chess.
Many people use their phones for several hours each day.
Object (indirect):
She read her daughter a bedtime story.
He gave his wife a phone for her birthday.
I made the teacher an offer to clean her car.
Complement:
She is very intelligent.
My brother is an engineer at Siemens.
This cheese smells bad.
Adjunct:
My father always drives very slowly.
Please send me a reply by the end of the week.
I have no time to play tennis as I have to study for the test tomorrow.
Note: I refer to 'sentences' throughout this article, but most of what I write applies also to 'finite clauses'.

The seven canonical sentence structures

According to A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language (page 721) there are seven basic (also known as default or canonical) sentence structures formed from a combination of two or more of the five constituents. These are:

  1. Subject-Verb (SV):
    • She laughed.
    • My goldfish has died.
  2. Subject-Verb-Object (SVO):
    • He collects stamps.
    • I bought a new phone.
  3. Subject-Verb-Complement (SVC):
    • She is a politician.
    • The cake smells delicious.
  4. Subject-Verb-Object-Object (SVOO):
    • She gave me a gift.
    • He taught the students mathematics.

      Note: me and the students are indirect objects; a gift and mathematics are direct objects.

  5. Subject-Verb-Adjunct (SVA):
    • My office is on the top floor.
    • The basketball court was behind the car park.
  6. Subject-Verb-Object-Adjunct (SVOA):
    • She put her phone on the bedside table.
    • I keep my precious bike in the hallway.
  7. Subject-Verb-Object-Complement (SVOC):
    • I found the movie boring.
    • They elected her captain of the chess team.

As you see, each of the seven constituent structures starts subject-verb (SV).

Note 1: Some grammarians prefer the term predicator to verb, since predicator is a function term in the same way as the other four constituent terms are functions, whereas verb is a word class, not a function.
Note 2: There are other very common structures, most of them with adjuncts in various positions in the sentence. In most cases, these adjuncts are optional elements - the sentence is still grammatical and makes sense if the adjunct is removed.

Conversely, in the SVOA structures exemplified by the Comprehensive Grammar the adjunct is a necessary element. For example, it is ungrammatical and nonsensical to say She put her phone. This is due to the nature of the verb to put.

Similarly, the verb to be in certain contexts needs an adjunct. For example, the adjunct in the garden in the SVA sentence I was in the garden.

Most other verbs do not function in the same way and can be followed by an optional adjunct. For example, the optional adjunct with my sister in I played chess with my sister.

Reader expectations

Native English readers intuitively expect the information presented to them will conform to one of the seven constituent orders above. For example, they expect the subject to be followed by a verb indicating what the subject does or is. They expect a transitive verb such as put to be followed immediately by an object. So, they will be surprised by I put in the dishwasher the plate.

But, as well as such expectations about the specific constituent order of a sentence, readers also expect that information will be presented to them according to certain more general organisational principles. These principles are explained in the next section.

General organisational principles

The four most important general organisational principles are topic-comment, given-new, end-focus and end-weight. Over stretches of text these principles combine to allow writers to control the information flow, so that the texts are perceived as being cohesive.

Note: Not all grammarians use the same terms as the four listed above. But there is broad agreement on the organisational principles that the terms entail.

- Topic-Comment

This is probably the most basic expectation that readers have about the structure of a sentence. Namely, that it will start with the topic (in other words, who or what the sentence will be about) and that it will be followed by information about the topic. This information is called the comment.

Note: Some grammarians use the terms theme-rheme or topic-focus instead of topic-comment.

In most cases, the topic of the sentence (i.e. the phrase or clause at the start of the sentence) will also be the grammatical subject. Here are three examples from above, in which the topic/grammatical subject is coloured red, and the comment is the rest of the sentence (the predicate).

In many cases, however, the writer will wish the topic of the sentence to be something other than the grammatical subject. A simple example is Next week my grandmother is arriving from Rome. The topic of the sentence, what the sentence is about, is Next week, and the comment (the rest of the sentence) tells us what is happening next week.

Here are two simple examples of where the topic/start of the sentence is not the grammatical subject. (The topic is coloured red and the grammatical subject green.)

Note: Putting an element other than the grammatical subject at the start of the sentence is called fronting. Fronting is not necessarily done in order to make the fronted element 'what the sentence is about'. It may also be done to give that element more emphasis or focus than it would have in its usual position in a sentence.

Fronting will be covered in detail later in the article.

In summary, the topic-comment structure is the primary organisational principle. It based on the fundamental expectation by readers of how writers should present information to them.

- Given-New

The second reader expectation, and consequently an organisational principle, is that given information should come before new information, generally at the start of the sentence. The concept of what can be categorized as given is somewhat vague. Indeed, not all grammarians like the term given and use alternatives such as discourse-old and common ground.

But basically, what is given is regarded as information that the reader has already been presented with or can be expected to infer from the context. A simple example of what has already been presented to the reader is the pronoun it at the start of the second sentence here:

It obviously refers to the watch and is therefore clearly given information.

In the following text, the second sentence starts with new information (namely, the postman):

The second sentence above will probably be regarded as less natural by many readers than:

Here's a slightly longer text in which each new sentence (after the first one) starts with new information:

The second world war began in September 1939. The invasion of Poland by German troops caused it. The Polish Socialist Party governed Poland at this time.

Many readers will find it, too, 'awkward' or unnatural without necessarily being able to identify why. The answer, of course, is that the second and third sentences contravene the given-new principle, and are thus found lacking in cohesion - a major advantage of given-new.

Here is the given-new version:

The second world war began in September 1939. It was caused by the invasion of Poland by German troops. At this time Poland was governed by the Polish Socialist Party.

Another example of what can be called given or discourse-old information is on a stool in I walked into the kitchen. On a stool was a book about cats. Everyone knows that kitchens usually have stools. This is the common ground; in other words, information that the reader can infer.

Compare this with I walked into the kitchen. On a bed was a book about cats. Most readers will probably find on a bed here odd† and classify it as new information in the way that on a stool is not.

Note: The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language (p1369) uses the word infelicitous to describe the on a bed sentence.
Important: The watch sentence above exemplifies how the given-new principle is certainly not (and cannot be) applied on all occasions. Typically, sentences that introduce a text or conversation will start with (or contain exclusively) new information.

So, the entire sentence My mother sent me a watch for Christmas is new information.

- End-Focus

The principle of end-focus is based on the fact that readers generally pay the greatest attention to the end of a sentence. They do this because they have internalised the principle of given-new. They correctly assume that the new information is likely to be the most important element in the sentence and should be the primary focus of their attention.

Note, however, that the principle of end-focus does not have to be in the context of a given-new sentence. For example:

The topic of the sentence is Inside our shed yesterday, which sets the scene, but the writer uses the principle of end-focus to indicate to the reader that the presence of the family of cats is the more important information

Fiction writers can use the principle of end-focus to create surprise or suspense by placing the grammatical subject (in red below) at the end. For example:

- End-Weight

End-weight is the last general organisational principle to be discussed here. The term has various interpretations. In one interpretation, weight is regarded as synonymous with importance. So we can see the link between end-weight and end-focus in the cat example above.

But the weight of an element according to a different, more common interpretation of this principle refers simply to the length of the element. It states the preference for the longest or most complex element in a sentence to appear last. Here is a simple example of a sentence that contravenes the end-weight principle:

The very 'light' particle on at the end of this sentence is infelicitous. Much better is to position the particle directly after the verb.

In the following example†, the writer has chosen to contravene the canonical subject-verb-object-complement SVOC order, since the object is very 'weighty' and the complement very 'light' (short).

The complement is shown in red and the object in green. The canonical SVOC order of the full sentence is very hard to process, since the reader has to hold a great deal of information in her head before understanding the message that the writer wishes to convey:

Note: The extract is from the Oxford Modern English Grammar (page 321).

-- Principle conflicts

It is not uncommon for general organisational principles to stand in conflict with each other. A typical conflict is between given-new and end-weight. Consider two consecutive sentences from the next section in the article.

They also choose non-canonical constructions in order to give focus or emphasis to a particular element in the sentence. Information packaging is the term for the use of specific techniques to achieve these two goals† and that result in non-canonical constructions.

The second sentence starts with new, not given, information. Namely, the term information packaging. Hence it contravenes the given-new principle. But it conforms to the end-weight principle in that it concludes with the 'weighty' and complex noun phrase complement:

...the term for the use of specific techniques to achieve these two goals and that result in non-canonical constructions

Alternatively, flouting the end-weight principle in order to conform to the given-new principle results in:

They also choose non-canonical constructions in order to give focus or emphasis to a particular element in the sentence. The term for the use of specific techniques to achieve these two goals and that result in non-canonical constructions is information packaging.
Note 1: The phrase these two goals in the second sentence of the text is an An anaphoric reference is a reference to a prior word or words in the text - usually in the previous sentence or clause.

Pronouns are a common anaphoric reference. For example, he in My brother's name is Mark; he is a teacher.

In the example text here, the phrase these two goals is an anaphoric reference to the goals of 'prioritizing general organisational principles' and 'giving focus or emphasis to a particular element in the sentence'.
anaphoric
reference to the passage before the text above. Anaphoric references are a clear indication that we are dealing with given information.
Note 2: In the case of the following SVC sentence from a Guardian article, the writer has prioritised end-focus over end-weight–with a 34-word subject followed by a 3-word predicate–in order to emphasise the complement a bagatelle.
"To lose 1-0 to a Rosenborg side 16 games into their domestic season via an added-time winner in what was United's first pre-season outing with a finishing XI that contained no recognised first-team squad members seems a bagatelle."
You can decide for yourself if this was a good decision.

Information packaging

So far, we have looked at the canonical constituent orders of sentences and at the general organisational principles or reader expectations of how information will be presented to them.

When these two come into conflict, it is usual for writers to prioritise the latter and write non-canonical constituent orders. They also choose non-canonical constructions in order to give focus or emphasis to a particular element in the sentence.

The term for the use of specific techniques to achieve these two goals and that result in non-canonical constructions is information packaging. The specific information packaging techniques are the subject of the next section of the article.

Note: Information packaging is the term used by The Cambridge Grammar of the English Language. Other grammars have alternative terms for the topic, such as information structure, information focus, emphasis and focus, among others.

- Fronting

The first information packaging technique is fronting (also called preposing). Fronting is placing a sentence element other than the grammatical subject at the start of the sentence. The particular element is in most cases an adjunct - either an adverbial phrase or an adverbial clause. We saw an example of this earlier. Namely:

Here are more fronted adjunct phrases or clauses:

Other types of fronted element include complements, objects, non-finite constructions and dependent clauses. When they start the sentence they attain a stronger emphasis and in many cases become the topic. Here are some examples:

- Inversion

In most cases, placing an element other than the subject at the front of the sentence does not change the constituent order of the rest of the sentence. There are, however, five types of fronting that do change the constituent order of the rest of the sentence, namely subject-verb (SV) inversion†.

Here are examples of the necessary SV inversion in each type. The verb-subject is shown in italics:

Note 1: Some grammars (e.g. Longman Student Grammar of Spoken and Written English) categorize inversion as an information packaging technique in its own right. Hence its inclusion here in a separate section.

††Note 2: Some grammarians (e.g. Peters in The Cambridge Dictionary of English Grammar) loosely designate the adverbs and adjectives in such constructions as 'comparative adverbs and adjectives'.

†††Note 3: In the middle of the field a large scarecrow with broomstick stood is also grammatical, but it contravenes the principles of end-focus and end-weight.

- Passive sentences

Writers use the passive for various reasons, but in the context of information packaging the most important one is to conform to the topic-comment and given-new principles.

In the simple active sentence Jack ate the pizza the topic is Jack. Using the passive equivalent enables the writer to make the pizza the topic: The pizza was eaten by Jack. This may be done, for example, to place the focus on which food was eaten by which person: The pizza was eaten by Jack; the hamburger by Jill.

As for conforming to the given-new principle, let's look first at a simple example:

The grammatical subject of the second sentence the police is new information. So the sentence contravenes the given-new principle.

The alternative version, using the passive, conforms to the principle. And, incidentally, it is another example of why the passive is better in this context. Namely, because the agent or doer of the action is obvious (it can only be the police who arrested him), and it can be omitted.

As a final example of the importance of the passive in information packaging, here again is the passivized version of the 'awkward' non-passive text from above about the start of the World War II:

It and Poland are given information, while caused by the invasion of Poland by German troops and was governed by the Polish Socialist Party are both new information. And in this case, they also both conform to the principles of end-focus and end-weight.

- Postposing

Postposing is placing a constituent later than its normal position in the sentence. For example, postposing the object in the canonical SVOA word order exemplified in I play chess every day. Certainly, the result of this postposing, the SVAO order, I play every day chess, is highly infelicitous.

However, there are many occasions on which such postposing is in fact desirable. This applies most typically when the postposing results in a sentence that conforms to the principle of end-weight and/or end-focus.

For example, in the standard SVOA word order sentence,

the object (in red) has a far greater weight than the adjunct (yesterday).

Note: Not only is it natural to place the object at the end of the sentence to comply with end-weight, but the canonical SVOA sentence above is ambiguous as to when yesterday refers to - the date of receipt of the letter or the date of the postal workers' strike?

Much better, in this case, is the postposing of the object in the non-canonical SVAO sentence:

Here are two more examples. The red words are the postposed object:

The postposing of the object is often also preferable even when the object has a lesser weight than the adjunct it switches places with. In this case, the writer prioritizes end-focus over end-weight:

Postposing of a complement is also possible but will often results in an infelicitous sentence. For example: ?He is at a private school a teacher and ?She wants to become in an orchestra a violinist.†

In summary, postposing is done to comply with the principle of end-weight or of end-focus or of both.

Note: A question mark at the start of a sentence is the conventional way that grammarians label the sentence as of questionable grammaticality or acceptability. Hence: ?He is at a private school a teacher.

An asterisk at the beginning of a sentence designates the sentence as ungrammatical. Hence: *An accident at the factory was from the section below on existential 'there'.

- Extraposition

The next information packaging technique is called extraposition. It's somewhat difficult to give the word a simple definition, so we will start with several examples:

The non-extraposed equivalents have a more basic SVC or SVO structure:

As you see, all the extraposed examples start with the non-referential pronoun it and end with a complement clause, thus complying with the principles of end-weight and end-focus.

It is no surprise, therefore, that extraposed sentences such as those above are more common than their non-extraposed equivalents.

Note that the previous sentence is itself an example of extrapositioning! Its non-extraposed equivalent is:

This version of the sentence, with its long subject, is more difficult to process, as well as contravening the principle of end-weight.

- Existential 'there'

The existential there is the there at the start of sentences such as There's a cat sitting on our neighbour's car and There was a lot of difficulty coming to an agreement.

Writers choose the existential 'there' since it complies with the given-new, end-weight and end-focus principles. For example, in the following sentences the words in italics are the semantic subject. In each case, this is new information; it has much more weight than its grammatical subject There and it thereby attains the reader's focus.

In some cases, there are existential and non-existential equivalents. For example: There's a fly in my soup and A fly is in my soup. But in many cases, such as in the three examples above, there is no acceptable or grammatical non-existential alternative:

The existential 'there' is a common and useful non-canonical construction.

- Cleft sentences

Cleft sentences are a fairly radical rearrangement of the constituents of a canonical sentence. Basically, they involve dividing the sentence into two parts, with the new part being fronted (preposed) or moved to the end of the sentence (postposed), thus emphasizing a particular element.

So, for example, take the simple sentence John ate the pizza. It can be divided into two parts in the following three ways:

The it-cleft above emphasises the semantic subject (John) as the pizza eater. This is one use of the structure; namely as a correction of a false claim. For example, the claim that Mary ate the pizza. The response could be expanded as: It was John who ate the pizza, not Mary. She ate the pasta. A similar it-cleft response to a false assumption about where the homework is done is It's in the kitchen that I do my homework (not in my bedroom).

The it-cleft is also common in neutral contexts to provide emphasis to the semantic subject. For example: It was in spring that we met for the first time. Or It is the head of school who will make the final decision.

Note: The it-cleft is a similar construction to the extraposition. For example, they both start with the dummy pronoun it as the subject.

The basic difference is that after the verb to-be the it-cleft has a noun phrase followed by relative clause, whereas the extraposition commonly has an adjectival phrase followed by a content clause.

The pseudo-cleft† (for example, Where I found the key was behind the fridge) allows the writer to place a special emphasis on the complement following the copula (the verb to be). In other words, behind the fridge in the above example.

Clearly, there is end-focus on behind the fridge in the canonical SVOA sentence I found the key behind the fridge. But the pseudo-cleft version amplifies that focus.

And in contrast to the SVOA sentence, the pseudo-cleft version allows compliance with the given-new principle. The given is shared information about the search for the lost key, and the new information is where it was found.

The reverse pseudo-cleft simply inverts the two parts of a regular pseudo-cleft. For example, Behind the fridge was where I found the key. This contravenes the given-new principle and gives greater emphasis to the Behind the fridge adjunct than it would have in the corresponding canonical SVOA version.

In summary, writers use the different cleft sentences to place emphasis on one or other of the sentence elements and to comply with one or more of the general principles of information packaging.

Note: The pseudo-cleft is also called the wh-cleft since these clefts start with one of the wh- words: who, what, why, when, etc.

- Topicalization

I have noted earlier that, in most cases, writers intend the first element in a sentence to be the topic of the sentence - what the sentence is about. Fronting is the most common way to establish an element other than the grammatical subject as the topic.

But there are some phrases that explicitly set the topic of the sentence. Here are some examples:

In each case above, the topicalization phrase can be considered to be given information, while the rest of the sentence (i.e. the comment) is new information.

Note: When written or spoken, the topicalized phrase at the start of the sentence is followed by a comma or a short pause respectively.

- Dislocation

Let's conclude with the specific information packaging technique called dislocation. There is left dislocation as exemplified in That new bike I bought last week, I've already crashed it three times. And right dislocation as in I've already crashed it three times, that new bike I bought last week.

In left-dislocation, the first clause is a clear case of explicit topicalization. For example, the topicalization of that book I lent you, in That book I lent you, can I have it back? Furthermore, the first clause is an example of compliance with the given-new principle, since the prior loaning of the book is common ground or shared knowledge between writer and reader.

The same content in right-dislocation is Can I have it back, that book I lent you? In this case, the second clause is needed in order to clarify the pronoun it in the first clause. Left dislocation overrides the given-new principle, but complies with end-focus.

Here are two more examples of each:

- left dislocation

- right dislocation

Note: Dislocation is far more common in spoken language than in written language.

Summary examples

Before concluding, here is a list of some of the ways that the unmarked† declarative SVOA sentence John ate pizza for dinner can be presented to the reader using the various information packaging techniques.

Unmarked: SVOA John ate pizza for dinner.
Passive Pizza was eaten for dinner by John.
Fronting For dinner John ate pizza.
Fronting Pizza John ate for dinner.
Postposing John ate for dinner pizza.
It-cleft It was John who ate pizza for dinner.
It-cleft It was pizza that John ate for dinner.
It-cleft It was for dinner that John ate pizza.
Pseudo-cleft What John ate for dinner was pizza.
Pseudo-cleft What John did was eat pizza for dinner.
Reverse pseudo-cleft Pizza was what John ate for dinner.
Reverse pseudo-cleft For dinner is when John ate pizza.
Reverse pseudo-cleft Eat pizza for dinner is what John did.
Reverse pseudo-cleft Pizza is what John ate for dinner.
Topicalization As for dinner, pizza is what John ate.
Topicalization As for pizza, for dinner is when John ate it.
Topicalization As for pizza, John ate it for dinner.
Left-dislocation Pizza, that's what John ate for dinner.
Left-dislocation Pizza, it was eaten for dinner by John.
Right-dislocation John ate it for dinner, pizza.

Hopefully, you now know enough information about information packaging to add one or two more sentences to the list above.

Note: Unmarked is the grammatical term for a canonical language structure, whereas marked is the term for structures that differ from canonical form in some way.

So, John ate pizza is unmarked, and Pizza was eaten by John is marked.

Sentence structures in this article

A large majority of the sentences in this long article comply with one of the seven canonical constituent structures listed at the beginning. And most of those that don't are examples of the fronting of an adjunct† (ASVO, ASVOC, etc).

So, clearly the priority for the English learner is to become very comfortable using those canonical structures, including the fronted adjunct variations. Nevertheless, there is great value in having knowledge of the marked structures analysed here and the reasons why one or other of them may be the best choice in a particular context.

Note: The fronting of an adjunct is such a common practice that such structures could justifiably also be called canonical or unmarked.

Conclusion

An understanding of the general principles for organising text sentence-by-sentence and knowledge of the specific techniques of information packaging to place a particular focus on one part of the sentence is important for English learners.†

Not only does such understanding and knowledge help learners to become effective writers in the various genres, but it also makes them more competent readers of the texts that others have written - in particular, how to detect structural differences from the norm and how to correctly interpret the writer's reasons for these differences.

Note: As a final example, the canonical (first) SVC sentence in the section above has a very long subject and a short predicate. It conforms to the topic-comment principle. But some readers may feel it would be better expressed in an extraposition to comply with the end-weight principle, as follows:

"It is important for English learners to have an understanding of the general principles for organising text sentence-by-sentence and knowledge of the specific techniques of information packaging to place a particular focus on one part of the sentence."

More on the topic

Elsewhere on this site are two video projects on the topics covered here: default constituent orders and information packaging principles and techniques.

You can quiz yourself on your knowledge and understanding of information packaging principles. And here you can practise identifying the specific information packaging techniques.

Finally, here is a more detailed look at the topic on an external website. The author gives particular attention to the concept of given as in given-new. His term for given is common ground, namely the common ground between the producer and the recipient of the message.

In contrast to this article, the author includes many examples of how information packaging (he calls it Information Structure) is performed in spoken language, where prosody (for example, intonation) can provide emphasis on a certain aspect of the text in a way that written language cannot.

Sources

The following grammar books were consulted in creating this article. Some of the example sentences here were taken from those sources:

Note that both the Longman Student Grammar of Spoken and Written English and A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language focus on information packaging in spoken language - an essential aspect of the topic that is not covered in this article.

The Cambridge Grammar of English and the Longman Student Grammar of Spoken and Written English cited above that are both pedagogic grammars aimed at English language learners. The other books are descriptive grammars.

The two pedagogic grammars are good at associating a particular marked sentence structure (i.e. as a result of the specific information packaging technique that has been applied) with one or other of the general organisational principles, such as given-new.

A final point

Grammarians frequently disagree on the classification and terminology of the various elements in a sentence. A particular issue concerns the distinction and overlap between complements and adjuncts.

For example, how should on the top floor in the sentence My office is on the top floor (see above) be classified? A Comprehensive Grammar of the English Language classifies it as an adjunct in the SVA sentence structure. But the phrase is clearly needed to 'complete the sentence', which fits the basic definition of a complement.

These kinds of issues are obviously of importance to linguists and grammarians. But for English language learners, the primary audience for this site, grammaticality and usage are fundamentally more important.

So, knowing when to use a pseudo-cleft and how to form it (for example, Where I found the key was behind the fridge) is more important than determining whether behind the fridge is an adjunct or a complement or both.